“References” in “What Is Cognitive Psychology?”
References
Ackley, D. H., Hinton, G. E., & Sejnowski, T. J. (1985). A learning algorithm for Boltzman machines. Cognitive Science, 9, 147–169.
Adams, F., & Aizawa, K. (2008). The bounds of cognition. Blackwell.
Adler, I. (1961). Thinking machines. Signet Science Library.
Agullo, M., Carlson, D., Clague, K., Ferrari, G., Ferrari, M., Yabuki, H., & Hempel, R. (2003). LEGO mindstorms masterpieces. Syngress Publishing.
Anderson, B. F. (1975). Cognitive psychology: The study of knowing, learning, and thinking. Academic Press.
Anderson, J. A., Silverstein, J. W., Ritz, S. A., & Jones, R. S. (1977). Distinctive features, categorical perception and probability learning: Some applications of a neural model. Psychological Review, 84, 413–451.
Anderson, J. R. (1980). Cognitive psychology and its implications. W. H. Freeman.
Anderson, J. R. (1983). The architecture of cognition. Harvard University Press.
Anderson, J. R. (1985). Cognitive psychology and its implications, 2nd edition. W. H. Freeman.
Anderson, J. R. (1990). The adaptive character of thought. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Anderson, J. R. (2000). Cognitive psychology and its implications (5th ed.). Worth Publishers.
Anderson, J. R. (2015). Cognitive psychology and its implications (8th ed.). Worth Publishers.
Anderson, J. R. (2020). Cognitive psychology and its implications (9th ed.). Worth Publishers.
Anderson, J. R., Bothell, D., Byrne, M. D., Douglass, S., Lebiere, C., & Qin, Y. L. (2004). An integrated theory of the mind. Psychological Review, 111(4), 1036–1060.
Anderson, J. R., & Bower, G. H. (1973). Human associative memory. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Anderson, J. R., & Matessa, M. (1997). A production system theory of serial memory. Psychological Review, 104(4), 728–748.
Anderson, M. L., Richardson, M. J., & Chemero, A. (2012). Eroding the boundaries of Cognition: Implications of embodiment. Topics in Cognitive Science, 4(4), 717–730. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1756-8765.2012.01211.x
Andresen, J. (1991). Skinner and Chomsky 30 years later on or, the return of the repressed. Behavior Analyst, 14(1), 49–60. https://doi.org/10.1007/bf03392552
Arrieta, A. B., Diaz-Rodriguez, N., Del Ser, J., Bennetot, A., Tabik, S., Barbado, A., . . . & Herrera, F. (2020). Explainable Artificial Intelligence (XAI): Concepts, taxonomies, opportunities and challenges toward responsible AI. Information Fusion, 58, 82–115. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.inffus.2019.12.012
Ashby, W. R. (1956). An introduction to cybernetics. Chapman & Hall.
Ashby, W. R. (1960). Design for a brain (2nd ed.). John Wiley & Sons.
Ashcraft, M. H. (1976). Priming and property dominance effects in semantic memory. Memory & Cognition, 4(5), 490–500. https://doi.org/10.3758/bf03213209
Ashcraft, M. H. (1978). Property dominance and typicality effects in property statement verification. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 17(2), 155–164. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0022-5371(78)90119-6
Baddeley, A. D. (1966). Short-term memory for word sequences as a function of acoustic semantic and formal similarity. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18, 362–365. https://doi.org/10.1080/14640746608400055
Baddeley, A. D. (1986). Working memory. Oxford University Press.
Baddeley, A. D. (1990). Human memory: Theory and practice. Allyn & Bacon.
Baddeley, A. D., & Dale, H. C. A. (1966). Effect of semantic similarity on retroactive interference in long- and short-term memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 5(5), 417–420. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0022-5371(66)80054-3
Bandettini, P. A. (2012). Twenty years of functional MRI: The science and the stories. Neuroimage, 62(2), 575–588. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.04.026
Bateson, G. (1972). Steps to an ecology of mind. Ballantine Books.
Bechtel, W., & Abrahamsen, A. (1991). Connectionism and the mind. Blackwell.
Bechtel, W., & Abrahamsen, A. A. (2002). Connectionism and the mind: Parallel processing, dynamics, and evolution in networks (2nd ed.). Blackwell.
Bell, D. A. (1962). Intelligent machines: An introduction to cybernetics. Blaisdell.
Bender, A., & Beller, S. (2012). Nature and culture of finger counting: Diversity and representational effects of an embodied cognitive tool. Cognition, 124(2), 156–182. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2012.05.005
Bengio, Y. (2009). Learning deep architectures for AI. Foundations and Trends in Machine Learning, 2(1), 1–127.
Bengio, Y., Courville, A., & Vincent, P. (2013). Representation learning: A review and new perspectives. IEEE Transactions on Pattern Analysis and Machine Intelligence, 35(8), 1798–1828. https://doi.org/10.1109/tpami.2013.50
Benjamin, L. T. (2019). A brief history of modern psychology (3rd ed.). Blackwell.
Bennett, M. R., Dennett, D., Hacker, P., & Searle, J. R. (2007). Neuroscience and philosophy: Brain, mind, and language. Columbia University Press.
Bennett, M. R., & Hacker, P. M. S. (2003). Philosophical foundations of neuroscience. Blackwell.
Bennett, M. R., & Hacker, P. M. S. (2013). History of cognitive neuroscience. Wiley-Blackwell.
Berkeley, E. C. (1949). Giant brains: Or, Machines that think. Wiley.
Berkeley, I. S. N., Dawson, M. R. W., Medler, D. A., Schopflocher, D. P., & Hornsby, L. (1995). Density plots of hidden value unit activations reveal interpretable bands. Connection Science, 7, 167–186.
Bertalanffy, L. V. (1967). Robots, men, and minds. Braziller.
Bever, T. G., Fodor, J. A., & Garrett, M. (1968). A formal limitation of associationism. In T. R. Dixon & D. L. Horton (Eds.), Verbal behavior and general behavior theory (pp. 582–585). Prentice-Hall.
Bharucha, J. J. (1999). Neural nets, temporal composites, and tonality. In D. Deutsch (Ed.), The psychology of music (2nd ed., pp. 413–440). Academic Press.
Biederman, I. (1987). Recognition by components: A theory of human image understanding. Psychological Review, 94, 115–147.
Bliss, T. V. P., & Lomo, T. (1973). Long-lasting potentiation of synaptic transmission in dentate area of anesthetized rabbit following stimulation of perforant path. Journal of Physiology—London, 232(2), 331–356.
Block, N. (1981). Imagery. MIT Press.
Bock, R. D., & Jones, L. V. (1968). The measurement and prediction of judgment and choice. Holden-Day.
Boden, M. (1977). Artificial intelligence and natural man. Basic Books.
Boole, G. (1854/2003). The laws of thought. Prometheus Books.
Boring, E. G. (1950). A history of experimental psychology. Appleton-Century-Crofts.
Bourne, L. E., Dominowski, R. L., & Loftus, E. F. (1979). Cognitive processes. Prentice-Hall.
Bousfield, W. A., & Cowan, T. M. (1964). Immediate memory spans for CVC trigrams. Journal of General Psychology, 70(2), 283–293. https://doi.org/10.1080/00221309.1964.9920599
Braisby, N., & Gellatly, A. (2012). Cognitive psychology (2nd ed.). Oxford University Press.
Braitenberg, V. (1984). Vehicles: Explorations in synthetic psychology. MIT Press.
Bransford, J. D., Barclay, J. R., & Franks, J. J. (1972). Sentence memory: Constructive versus interpretive approach. Cognitive Psychology, 3(2), 193–209.
Breazeal, C. (2003). Toward sociable robots. Robotics and Autonomous Systems, 42(3–4), 167–175. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0921-8890(02)00373-1
Breazeal, C. (2004). Social interactions in HRI: The robot view. IEEE Transactions on Systems Man and Cybernetics Part C—Applications and Reviews, 34(2), 181–186. https://doi.org/10.1109/tsmcc.2004.826268
Breazeal C., Dautenhahn K., Kanda T. (2016) Social Robotics. In: Siciliano B., Khatib O. (eds) Springer Handbook of Robotics. Springer Handbooks. Springer.
Breazeal, C., Gray, J., & Berlin, M. (2009). An embodied cognition approach to mindreading skills for socially intelligent robots. International Journal of Robotics Research, 28(5), 656–680. https://doi.org/10.1177/0278364909102796
Breazeal, C. L. (2002). Designing sociable robots. MIT Press.
Brentano, F. C. (1874/1995). Psychology from an empirical standpoint (Paperback ed.). Routledge.
Broadbent, D. E. (1958). Perception and communication. Pergamon Press.
Bronowski, J. (1973). The ascent of man. British Broadcasting Corporation.
Brooks, R. A. (1991). Intelligence without representation. Artificial Intelligence, 47, 139–159.
Brooks, R. A. (1997). The Cog project. Journal of the Robotics Society of Japan, 15(7), 969–970.
Brooks, R. A. (1999). Cambrian intelligence: The early history of the new AI. MIT Press.
Brooks, R. A. (2002). Flesh and machines: How robots will change us. Pantheon Books.
Brooks, R. A., Breazeal, C., Marjanovic, M., Scasselati, B., & Williamson, M. M. (1999). The Cog project: Building a humanoid robot. In C. Nehaniv. (Ed.), Computation for metaphors, analogy, and agents. CMAA 1998. Lecture notes in computer science, vol. 1562. Springer.
Brooks, R. A., & Stein, L. A. (1994). Building brains for bodies. Autonomous Robots, 1(1), 7–25. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00735340
Brown, R. E., & Milner, P. M. (2003). Timeline—The legacy of Donald O. Hebb: More than the Hebb synapse. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 4(12), 1013–1019. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn1257
Brown, T. H. (1990). Hebbian synapses: Biophysical mechanisms and algorithms. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 13, 475–511.
Bruner, J. S. (1957). On perceptual readiness. Psychological Review, 64, 123–152.
Bruner, J. S. (1990). Acts of meaning. Harvard University Press.
Bruner, J. S. (1992). Another look at New Look 1. American Psychologist, 47(6), 780–783.
Bruner, J. S., Postman, L., & Rodrigues, J. (1951). Expectation and the perception of color. American Journal of Psychology, 64(2), 216–227.
Cabeza, R., & Nyberg, L. (1997). Imaging cognition: An empirical review of PET studies with normal subjects. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 9(1), 1–26.
Cabeza, R., & Nyberg, L. (2000). Imaging cognition II: An empirical review of 275 PET and fMRI studies. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12(1), 1–47. https://doi.org/10.1162/08989290051137585
Calvo, P., & Gomila, A. (2008). Handbook of cognitive science: An embodied approach. Elsevier.
Campbell, M., Hoane, A. J., & Hsu, F. H. (2002). Deep Blue. Artificial Intelligence, 134(1–2), 57–83. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0004-3702(01)00129-1
Carruthers, P. (2006). The architecture of the mind: Massive modularity and the flexibility of thought. Oxford University Press.
Cermak, L. S., & Craik, F. I. M. (1979). Levels of processing in human memory. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Chemero, A. (2000). Anti-representationalism and the dynamical stance. Philosophy of Science, 67(4), 625–647. https://doi.org/10.1086/392858
Chemero, A. (2009). Radical embodied cognitive science. MIT Press.
Ching, T., Himmelstein, D. S., Beaulieu-Jones, B. K., Kalinin, A. A., Do, B. T., Way, G. P., . . . & Greene, C. S. (2018). Opportunities and obstacles for deep learning in biology and medicine. Journal of the Royal Society Interface, 15(141). https://doi.org/10.1098/rsif.2017.0387
Chomsky, N. (1957). Syntactic structures (2nd ed.). Mouton de Gruyter.
Chomsky, N. (1959). A review of B. F. Skinner’s Verbal behavior. Language, 35, 26–58.
Chomsky, N. (1965). Aspects of the theory of syntax. MIT Press.
Chomsky, N. (1966). Cartesian linguistics: A chapter in the history of rationalist thought. Harper & Row.
Chomsky, N. (1995). The minimalist program. MIT Press.
Chomsky, N., Mukherji, N., Patnaik, B. N., & Agnihotri, R. K. (2000). The architecture of language. Oxford University Press.
Chrisomalis, S. (2013). Constraint, cognition, and written numeration. Pragmatics & Cognition, 21(3), 552–572. https://doi.org/10.1075/pc.21.3.08chr
Churchland, P. M. (2005). Cleansing science. Inquiry—An Interdisciplinary Journal of Philosophy, 48(5), 464–477. https://doi.org/10.1080/00201740500242001
Churchland, P. M., & Churchland, P. S. (1990). Could a machine think? Scientific American, 262, 32–37.
Churchland, P. S., Koch, C., & Sejnowski, T. J. (1990). What is computational neuroscience? In E. L. Schwartz (Ed.), Computational neuroscience (pp. 46–55). MIT Press.
Clark, A. (1989). Microcognition. MIT Press.
Clark, A. (1993). Associative engines. MIT Press.
Clark, A. (1997). Being there: Putting brain, body, and world together again. MIT Press.
Clark, A. (1999). An embodied cognitive science? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 3(9), 345–351.
Clark, A. (2003). Natural-born cyborgs. Oxford University Press.
Clark, A. (2008). Supersizing the mind: Embodiment, action, and cognitive extension. Oxford University Press.
Clark, A. (2016). Surfing uncertainty: Prediction, action, and the embodied mind. Oxford University Press.
Clark, A., & Chalmers, D. (1998). The extended mind (active externalism). Analysis, 58(1), 7–19.
Cofer, C. N. (1978). Origins of the Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 17(1), 113–126. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0022-5371(78)90573-x
Cohen, J. D., Dunbar, K., & McClelland, J. L. (1991). On the control of automatic processes: A parallel distributed processing account of the Stroop effect. Psychological Review, 97, 332–361.
Colby, K. M., Hilf, F. D., Weber, S., & Kraemer, H. C. (1972). Turing-like indistinguishability tests for validation of a computer simulation of paranoid processes. Artificial Intelligence, 3(2), 199–221.
Collins, A. M., & Loftus, E. F. (1975). Spreading activation theory of semantic processing. Psychological Review, 82(6), 407–428. https://doi.org/10.1037//0033-295x.82.6.407
Collins, A. M., & Quillian, M. R. (1969). Retrieval time from semantic memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 8, 240–247.
Coltheart, M., Rastle, K., Perry, C., Langdon, R., & Ziegler, J. (2001). DRC: A dual route cascaded model of visual word recognition and reading aloud. Psychological Review, 108(1), 204–256. https://doi.org/10.1037//0033-295x.108.1.204
Conrad, R. (1964). Acoustic confusions in immediate memory. British Journal of Psychology, 55(1), 75–84.
Conway, F., & Siegelman, J. (2005). Dark hero of the information age: In search of Norbert Wiener, the father of cybernetics. Basic Books.
Conway, M. A. (1997). Cognitive models of memory (1st MIT Press ed.). MIT Press.
Cook, V. J., & Newson, M. (1996). Chomsky’s universal grammar: An introduction. Blackwell.
Cooper, L. A., & Shepard, R. N. (1973a). Chronometric studies of the rotation of mental images. In W. G. Chase (Ed.), Visual information processing (pp. 75–176). Academic Press.
Cooper, L. A., & Shepard, R. N. (1973b). The time required to prepare for a rotated stimulus. Memory & Cognition, 1(3), 246–250.
Cooter, R. (2014). Neural veils and the will to historical critique: Why historians of science need to take the neuro-turn seriously. Isis, 105(1), 145–154. https://doi.org/10.1086/675556
Cox, R. R., & Evarts, E. V. (1961). An evoked response detector. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 13(3), 478–480 https://doi.org/10.1016/0013-4694(61)90023-2
Craik, F. I. M. (2002). Levels of processing: Past, present . . . and future? Memory, 10(5–6), 305–318. https://doi.org/10.1080/09658210244000135
Craik, F. I. M., & Lockhart, R. S. (1972). Levels of processing: A framework for memory research. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 11, 671–684.
Cramer, P. (1968). Word association. Academic Press.
Cummins, R. (1975). Functional analysis. Journal of Philosophy, 72, 741–760.
Cummins, R. (1983). The nature of psychological explanation. MIT Press.
Cummins, R. (1989). Meaning and mental representation. MIT Press.
Dartnall, H. J. A., Bowmaker, J. K., & Molino, J. D. (1983). Human visual pigments: Microspectrophotometric results from the eyes of seven persons. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B—Biological Sciences, 220, 115–130.
Dawson, G. D. (1954). A summation technique for the detection of small evoked potentials. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 6(1), 65–84. https://doi.org/10.1016/0013-4694(54)90007-3
Dawson, M. R. W. (1991). The how and why of what went where in apparent motion: Modeling solutions to the motion correspondence process. Psychological Review, 98, 569–603.
Dawson, M. R. W. (1998). Understanding cognitive science. Blackwell.
Dawson, M. R. W. (2004). Minds and machines: Connectionism and psychological modeling. Blackwell.
Dawson, M. R. W. (2009). Computation, cognition—and connectionism. In D. Dedrick & L. Trick (Eds.), Cognition, computation, and Pylyshyn (pp. 175–199). MIT Press.
Dawson, M. R. W. (2013). Mind, body, world: Foundations of cognitive science. Athabasca University Press.
Dawson, M. R. W. (2018). Connectionist representations of tonal music: Discovering musical patterns by interpreting artificial neural networks. Athabasca University Press.
Dawson, M. R. W. (2022). Probability learning by perceptrons and people. Comparative Cognition and Behavior Reviews, 15 (monograph), 1–188.
Dawson, M. R. W., Dupuis, B., & Wilson, M. (2010). From bricks to brains: The embodied cognitive science of LEGO robots. Athabasca University Press.
Dawson, M. R. W., & Harshman, R. A. (1986). The multidimensional analysis of asymmetries in alphabetic confusion matrices: Evidence for global-to-local and local-to-global processing. Perception & Psychophysics, 40(6), 370–383. https://doi.org/10.3758/bf03208196
Dawson, M. R. W., Medler, D. A., McCaughan, D. B., Willson, L., & Carbonaro, M. (2000). Using extra output learning to insert a symbolic theory into a connectionist network. Minds and Machines, 10, 171–201.
Dawson, M. R. W., Perez, A., & Sylvestre, S. (2020). Artificial neural networks solve musical problems with Fourier phase spaces. Scientific Reports, 10(1), 7151. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-64229-4
de Oliveira, G. S., Raja, V., & Chemero, A. (2019). Radical embodied cognitive science and “Real Cognition.” Synthese (198), 1–22. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11229-019-02475-4
Deeks, A. (2019). The judicial demand for explainable artificial intelligence. Columbia Law Review, 119(7), 1829–1850.
Deese, J. (1965). The structure of associations in language and thought. Johns Hopkins University Press.
Deese, J., & Hulse, S. H. (1967). The psychology of learning (3rd ed.). McGraw-Hill.
Dehaene, S. (2011). The number sense: How the mind creates mathematics (Rev. and updated ed.). Oxford University Press.
Dennett, D. C. (2007). Philosophy as naive anthropology: Comment on Bennett and Hacker. In M. R. Bennett, D. C. Dennett, P. M. S. Hacker, & J. R. Searle (Eds.), Neuroscience and Philosophy: Brain, Mind, and Language (pp. 73–96). Columbia University Press.
Descartes, R. (1637/1960). Discourse on method and meditations. Bobbs-Merrill.
Dodd, D. H., & White, R. M. (1980). Cognition: Mental structures and processes. Allyn & Bacon.
Doidge, N. (2007). The brain that changes itself: Stories of personal triumph from the frontiers of brain science. Viking.
Donders, F. C. (1869/1969). On the speed of mental processes. Acta Psychologica, 30, 412–431. https://doi.org/10.1016/0001-6918(69)90065-1
Dove, G. (2014). Thinking in words: Language as an embodied medium of thought. Topics in Cognitive Science, 6(3), 371–389. https://doi.org/10.1111/tops.12102
Dreyfus, H. L. (1967). Why computers must have bodies in order to be intelligent. Review of Metaphysics, 21(1), 13–32.
Dreyfus, H. L. (1972). What computers can’t do: A critique of artificial reason. Harper & Row.
Dreyfus, H. L. (1992). What computers still can’t do. MIT Press.
Dreyfus, H. L., & Dreyfus, S. E. (1988). Making a mind versus modeling the brain: Artificial intelligence back at the branchpoint. In S. Graubard (Ed.), The artificial intelligence debate (pp 15–44). MIT Press.
Dudai, Y. (1989). The neurobiology of memory. Oxford University Press.
Duncker, K. (1945). On problem-solving. Psychological Monographs, 58(5), 1–112.
Dutton, J. M., & Briggs, W. G. (1971). Simulation model construction. In J. M. Dutton & W. H. Starbuck (Eds.), Computer simulation of human behavior (pp. 103–126). John Wiley & Sons.
Dutton, J. M., & Starbuck, W. H. (1971). Computer simulation of human behavior. John Wiley & Sons.
Dyer, F. N. (1973). The Stroop phenomenon and its use in study of perceptual, cognitive, and response processes. Memory & Cognition, 1(2), 106–120. https://doi.org/10.3758/bf03198078
Eichenbaum, H. (2002). The cognitive neuroscience of memory: An introduction. Oxford University Press.
Elman, J. L., Bates, E. A., Johnson, M. H., Karmiloff-Smith, A., Parisi, D., & Plunkett, K. (1996). Rethinking innateness. MIT Press.
Erhan, D., Courville, A., & Bengio, Y. (2010). Understanding representations learned in deep architectures. Technical Report 1355, Departement d’informatique et recherche operationnelle, Université de Montréal.
Ericsson, K. A., & Simon, H. A. (1984). Protocol analysis: Verbal reports as data. MIT Press.
Ericsson, K. A., & Simon, H. A. (1993). Protocol analysis (Rev. ed.). MIT Press.
Estes, W. K. (1975). Some targets for mathematical psychology. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 12, 263–282.
Eysenck, M. W., & Keane, M. T. (2020). Cognitive psychology: A student’s handbook (8th ed.). Routledge.
Farah, M. J. (1994). Neuropsychological inference with an interactive brain: A critique of the “locality” assumption. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 17, 43–104.
Farina, M. V. (1970). Flowcharting. Prentice-Hall.
Farmer, T. A., & Matlin, M. W. (2019). Cognition (10th ed.). Wiley.
Feigenbaum, E. A., & Feldman, J. (1963). Computers and thought. McGraw-Hill.
Feigenbaum, E. A., & McCorduck, P. (1983). The fifth generation. Addison-Wesley.
Ferrari, G. (2006). Masterpiece 3—The LEGO Turing machine. In K. Clague (Ed.), Classic LEGO mindstorms projects and software tools (pp. 105–150). Syngress.
Figdor, C. (2013). What is the “cognitive” in cognitive neuroscience? Neuroethics, 6(1), 105–114. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12152-012-9157-5
Finke, R.A., & Pinker, S. (1982). Spontaneous imagery scanning in mental extrapolation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 8, 142-147.
Fischer, M. H., & Brugger, P. (2011). When digits help digits: Spatial-numerical associations point to finger counting as prime example of embodied cognition. Frontiers in Psychology, 2. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00260
Fischer, M. H., & Zwaan, R. A. (2008). Embodied language: A review of the role of the motor system in language comprehension. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61(6), 825–850. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470210701623605
Fiske, S. T., & Taylor, S. E. (2020). Social cognition: From brains to culture (4th ed.). SAGE Publications.
Flanagan, O. J. (1984). The science of the mind. MIT Press.
Fodor, J. A. (1968). Psychological explanation: An introduction to the philosophy of psychology. Random House.
Fodor, J. A. (1975). The language of thought. Harvard University Press.
Fodor, J. A. (1983). The modularity of mind. MIT Press.
Fodor, J. A., & Pylyshyn, Z. W. (1988). Connectionism and cognitive architecture. Cognition, 28(1–2), 3–71.
Ford, K. M., & Pylyshyn, Z. W. (1996). The robot’s dilemma revisited: The frame problem in artificial intelligence. Ablex.
Forte, A. (1973). The structure of atonal music. Yale University Press.
Frisch, S. (2014). How cognitive neuroscience could be more biological—and what it might learn from clinical neuropsychology. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2014.00541
Fukushima, K. (1986). A neural network model for selective attention in visual pattern recognition. Biological Cybernetics, 55, 5–15.
Gallese, V., & Goldman, A. (1998). Mirror neurons and the simulation theory of mind-reading. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 2(12), 493–501.
Gallese, V., Keysers, C., & Rizzolatti, G. (2004). A unifying view of the basis of social cognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8(9), 396–403.
Gallese, V., & Sinigaglia, C. (2011). What is so special about embodied simulation? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15(11), 512–519. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2011.09.003
Gardner, H. (1984). The mind’s new science. Basic Books.
Gates, A. I. (1916). The mnemonic span for visual and auditory digits. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1, 393–403. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0071265
Gawehn, E., Hiss, J. A., & Schneider, G. (2016). Deep learning in drug discovery. Molecular Informatics, 35(1), 3–14. https://doi.org/10.1002/minf.201501008
Gentner, D., Holyoak, K. J., & Kokinov, B. N. (2001). The analogical mind: Perspectives from cognitive science. MIT Press.
Gerrissen, J. F. (1991). On the network-based emulation of human visual search. Neural Networks, 4, 543–564.
Gibson, J. J. (1979). The ecological approach to visual perception. Houghton Mifflin.
Gick, M. L., & Holyoak, K. J. (1980). Analogical problem solving. Cognitive Psychology, 12(3), 306–355. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0285(80)90013-4
Glanzer, M., & Cunitz, A. R. (1966). Two storage mechanisms in free recall. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 5(4), 351–360.
Glenberg, A. M., Witt, J. K., & Metcalfe, J. (2013). From the revolution to embodiment: 25 years of cognitive psychology. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 8(5), 573–585. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691613498098
Gluck, M. A., & Myers, C. (2001). Gateway to memory: An introduction to neural network modeling of the hippocampus and learning. MIT Press.
Goh, G. B., Hodas, N. O., & Vishnu, A. (2017). Deep learning for computational chemistry. Journal of Computational Chemistry, 38(16), 1291–1307. https://doi.org/10.1002/jcc.24764
Gold, E. M. (1967). Language identification in the limit. Information and Control, 10, 447–474.
Goldstein, E. B. (2011). Cognitive psychology: Connecting mind, research, and everyday experience (3rd ed.). Cengage Learning.
Goldstein, E. B. (2015). Cognitive psychology: Connecting mind, research, and everyday experience (4th ed., student ed.). Cengage Learning.
Goldstine, H. H. (1993). The computer—From Pascal to von Neumann. Princeton University Press.
Goldstine, H. H., & von Neumann, J. (1947). Planning and coding of problems for an electronic computing instrument (Vol. 1, Part 2, pp. 1–194). Institute for Advanced Study, Princeton University.
Goss, A. E., & Nodine, C. F. (1965). Paired-associates learning: The role of meaningfulness, similarity, and familiarization. Academic Press.
Grasse, P. P. (1959). La reconstruction du nid et les coordinations interindividuelles chez Bellicositermes natalensis et Cubitermes sp. la théorie de la stigmergie: Essai d’interprétation du comportement des termites constructeurs. Insectes Sociaux, 6(1), 41–80.
Gregory, R. L. (1970). The intelligent eye. Weidenfeld & Nicolson.
Gregory, R. L. (1978). Eye and brain. McGraw-Hill.
Grier, D.A. (2013). Edward Feigenbaum. IEEE Annals of the History of Computing, 35(4), 74-81. doi.org/10.1109/MAHC.2013.49
Grenville, B. (2001). The uncanny: Experiments in cyborg culture. Vancouver Art Gallery; Arsenal Pulp Press.
Grey Walter, W. (1963). The living brain. W. W. Norton.
Griffith, N., & Todd, P. M. (1999). Musical networks: Parallel distributed perception and performance. MIT Press.
Groome, D. (2014). An introduction to cognitive psychology: Processes and disorders (3rd ed.). Psychology Press, Taylor & Francis Group.
Haberlandt, K. (1994). Cognitive psychology. Allyn & Bacon.
Hanson, S. J., & Burr, D. J. (1990). What connectionist models learn: Learning and representation in connectionist networks. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 13, 471–518.
Haugeland, J. (1985). Artificial intelligence: The very idea. MIT Press.
Hebb, D. O. (1949). The organization of behavior: A neuropsychological theory. Wiley.
Hecht-Nielsen, R. (1987). Neurocomputing. Addison-Wesley.
Heidbreder, E. (1933). Seven psychologies (Student’s ed.). Appleton-Century-Crofts.
Heinze, H. J., Mangun, G. R., Burchert, W., Hinrichs, H., Scholz, M., Munte, T. F., . . . & Hillyard, S. A. (1994). Combined spatial and temporal imaging of brain activity during visual selective attention in humans. Nature, 372(6506), 543–546. https://doi.org/10.1038/372543a0
Helmholtz, H. (1868/1968). The recent progress of the theory of vision. In R. M. Warren & R. P. Warren (Eds.), Helmholtz on perception: Its Physiology and Development (pp. 61–136). John Wiley & Sons.
Hilgard, E. R., & Marquis, D. G. (1940). Conditioning and learning. Appleton-Century.
Hillis, W. D. (1998). The pattern on the stone. Basic Books.
Hinton, G. E. (2007). Learning multiple layers of representation. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(10), 428–434. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2007.09.004
Hinton, G. E., & Anderson, J. A. (1981). Parallel models of associative memory. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Hinton, G. E., Osindero, S., & Teh, Y. (2006). A fast learning algorithm for deep belief nets. Neural Computation, 18(7), 1527–1554. https://doi.org/10.1162/neco.2006.18.7.1527
Hinton, G. E., & Salakhutdinov, R. R. (2006). Reducing the dimensionality of data with neural networks. Science, 313(5786), 504–507. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1127647
Hobbes, T. (1651/1967). Hobbes’s leviathan. Clarendon Press.
Hodges, A. (1983). Alan Turing: The enigma of intelligence. Unwin Paperbacks.
Hofstadter, D. R. (1979). Godel, Escher, Bach: An eternal golden braid. Basic Books.
Holyoak, K. J., & Thagard, P. (1995). Mental leaps: Analogy in creative thought. MIT Press.
Hopfield, J. J. (1982). Neural networks and physical systems with emergent collective computational abilities. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 79, 2554–2558.
Hopfinger, J. B., Buonocore, M. H., & Mangun, G. R. (2000). The neural mechanisms of top-down attentional control. Nature Neuroscience, 3(3), 284–291.
Horgan, T., & Tienson, J. (1996). Connectionism and the philosophy of psychology. MIT Press.
Hubel, D. H., & Wiesel, T. N. (1959). Receptive fields of single neurones in the cat’s striate cortex. Journal of Physiology, 148, 574–591.
Hubel, D. H., & Wiesel, T. N. (1962). Receptive fields, binocular interaction and functional architecture in the cat’s visual cortex. Journal of Physiology, 160, 106–154.
Hull, C. L., Hovland, C. I., Ross, R. T., Hall, M., Perkins, D. T., & Fitch, F. B. (1940). Mathematico deductive theory of rote learning: A study in scientific methodology. Yale University Press.
Hunt, E. (1971). What kind of computer is man? Cognitive Psychology, 2(1), 57–98. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0285(71)90003-x
Hurley, S. (2001). Perception and action: Alternative views. Synthese, 129(1), 3–40.
Hutchins, E. (1995). Cognition in the wild. MIT Press.
James, W. (1890). The principles of psychology, volume one. Dover Publications.
Jensen, A. R., & Rohwer, W. D. (1966). Stroop color-word test: A review. Acta Psychologica, 25(1), 36–93. https://doi.org/10.1016/0001-6918(66)90004-7
Jevons, W. S. (1870). On the mechanical performance of logical inference. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, 160, 497–518.
Joanisse, M. F., & McClelland, J. L. (2015). Connectionist perspectives on language learning, representation and processing. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 6(3), 235–247. https://doi.org/10.1002/wcs.1340
Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1983). Mental models. Harvard University Press.
Jones, E. G., & Mendell, L. M. (1999). Assessing the decade of the brain. Science, 284(5415), 739. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.284.5415.739
Kahneman, D., Treisman, A., & Gibbs, B. J. (1992). The reviewing of object files: Object-specific integration of information. Cognitive Psychology, 24(2), 175–219.
Kamilaris, A., & Prenafeta-Boldu, F. X. (2018). Deep learning in agriculture: A survey. Computers and Electronics in Agriculture, 147, 70–90. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.compag.2018.02.016
Kellogg, R. T. (1995). Cognitive psychology. SAGE Publications.
Kidder, T. (1981). The soul of a new machine. Avon Books.
Kirsh, D. (1995). The intelligent use of space. Artificial Intelligence, 73(1–2), 31–68. https://doi.org/10.1016/0004-3702(94)00017-u
Klein, R. M. (1999). The Hebb legacy. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 53(1), 1–3.
Knapp, T. J. (1985). Contributions to the history of psychology: Moore, T. V., and his “Cognitive Psychology” of 1939. Psychological Reports, 57(3), 1311–1316. https://doi.org/10.2466/pr0.1985.57.3f.1311
Köhler, W. (1947). Gestalt psychology, an introduction to new concepts in modern psychology. Liveright.
Köhler, W. (1925/2018). The mentality of apes (E. Winter, Trans.). Routledge.
Kohonen, T. (1977). Associative memory: A system-theoretical approach. Springer-Verlag.
Kok, A. (2020). Functions of the brain: A conceptual approach to cognitive neuroscience. Routledge.
Kolb, B. (1995). Brain plasticity and behavior. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Kosslyn, S. M. (1980). Image and mind. Harvard University Press.
Kosslyn, S. M. (1987). Seeing and imagining in the cerebral hemispheres: A computational approach. Psychological Review, 94(2), 148–175.
Kosslyn, S. M. (1994). Image and brain. MIT Press.
Kosslyn, S. M., Ball, T. M., & Reiser, B. J. (1978). Visual images preserve metric spatial information: Evidence from studies of image scanning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 4(1), 47–60.
Kosslyn, S. M., Brunn, J., Cave, K. R., & Wallach, R. W. (1984). Individual differences in mental imagery ability: A computational analysis. Cognition, 18(1–3), 195–243.
Kosslyn, S. M., Farah, M. J., Holtzman, J. D., & Gazzaniga, M. S. (1985). A computational analysis of mental image generation: Evidence from functional dissociations in split-brain patients. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 114(3), 311–341.
Kosslyn, S. M., & Shwartz, S. P. (1977). A simulation of visual imagery. Cognitive Science, 1, 265–295.
Kosslyn, S. M., Thompson, W. L., & Ganis, G. (2006). The case for mental imagery. Oxford University Press.
Kruschke, J. K. (1992). ALCOVE: An exemplar-based connectionist model of category learning. Psychological Review, 99, 22–44.
Külpe, O., & Titchener, E. B. (1895). Outlines of psychology, based upon the results of experimental investigation. Macmillan.
Kunda, Z. (1999). Social cognition: Making sense of people. MIT Press.
Kurzweil, R. (1990). The age of intelligent machines. MIT Press.
La Mettrie, J. O. D. (1750). Man a machine (2nd ed.). Printed for G. Smith.
Lachman, R., Lachman, J. L., & Butterfield, E. C. (1979). Cognitive psychology and information processing. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Laird, J. E., Newell, A., & Rosenbloom, P. S. (1987). Soar: An architecture for general intelligence. Artificial Intelligence, 33(1), 1–64. https://doi.org/10.1016/0004-3702(87)90050-6
Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M. (1999). Philosophy in the flesh: The embodied mind and its challenge to Western thought. Basic Books.
Lakoff, G., & Núñez, R. E. (2000). Where mathematics comes from: How the embodied mind brings mathematics into being. Basic Books.
Langenecker, S. A., Nielson, K. A., & Rao, S. M. (2004). fMRI of healthy older adults during Stroop interference. Neuroimage, 21(1), 192–200. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.08.027
Larochelle, H., Mandel, M., Pascanu, R., & Bengio, Y. (2012). Learning algorithms for the classification restricted Boltzmann machine. Journal of Machine Learning Research, 13, 643–669.
Lauterbur, P. C. (1973). Image formation by induced local interactions: Examples employing nuclear magnetic resonance. Nature, 242(5394), 190–191. https://doi.org/10.1038/242190a0
Leahey, T. H. (1987). A history of psychology (2nd ed.). Prentice-Hall.
Leahey, T. H. (1992). The mythical revolutions of American psychology. American Psychologist, 47(2), 308–318.
LeCun, Y., Bengio, Y., & Hinton, G. (2015). Deep learning. Nature, 521(7553), 436–444. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature14539
Leighton, J. P., & Sternberg, R. J. (2004). The nature of reasoning. Cambridge University Press.
Lewandowsky, S. (1993). The rewards and hazards of computer simulations. Psychological Science, 4, 236–243.
Lewandowsky, S., & Dunbar, K. (1983). Cognitive psychology: A comparative review of textbooks. American Journal of Psychology, 96(3), 391–403. https://doi.org/10.2307/1422320
Lippmann, R. P. (1989). Pattern classification using neural networks. IEEE Communications Magazine, November, 47–64.
Livingstone, M., & Hubel, D. (1988). Segregation of form, color, movement and depth: Anatomy, physiology, and perception. Science, 240 (4853), 740–750.
Locke, J. (1706/1977). An essay concerning human understanding. J. M. Dent & Sons.
Lockhart, R. S., & Craik, F. I. M. (1990). Levels of processing: A retrospective commentary on a framework for memory research. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 44(1), 87–112. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0084237
Lorayne, H., & Lucas, J. (1974). The memory book. Stein & Day.
Luce, R. D. (1986). Response times: Their role in inferring elementary mental organization. Oxford University Press.
Lynch, G. (1986). Synapses, circuits, and the beginnings of memory. MIT Press.
Macleod, C. M. (1991). Half a century of research on the Stroop effect: An integrative review. Psychological Bulletin, 109(2), 163–203. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.109.2.163
Macleod, C. M. (2015). Attention: Beyond Stroop’s (1935) colour-word interference phenomenon. In D. Groome & M. W. Eysenck (Eds.), Cognitive psychology: Revisiting the classic studies (pp. 60–70). SAGE Publications.
Mandler, G. (2002). Origins of the cognitive (r)evolution. Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences, 38(4), 339–353. https://doi.org/10.1002/jhbs.10066
Mangun, G. R., Buonocore, M. H., Girelli, M., & Jha, A. P. (1998). ERP and fMRI measures of visual spatial selective attention. Human Brain Mapping, 6(5–6), 383–389. https://doi.org/10.1002/(sici)1097-0193(1998)6:5/6<383::aid-hbm10>3.3.co;2-q
Manis, M. (1971). An introduction to cognitive psychology. Brooks/Cole.
Marek, P., & Griggs, R. A. (2001). Useful analyses for selecting a cognitive psychology textbook. Teaching of Psychology, 28(1), 40–44.
Marquand, A. (1885). A new logical machine. Proceedings of the American Academy of Arts and Sciences, 21, 303–307.
Marr, D. (1982). Vision. W. H. Freeman.
Martindale, C. (1991). Cognitive psychology: A neural-network approach. Brooks/Cole.
Martinez, J. L., & Derrick, B. E. (1996). Long-term potentiation and learning. Annual Review of Psychology, 47, 173–203.
Martinez, J. L., & Kesner, R. (1998). Neurobiology of learning and memory. Academic Press.
Maunsell, J. H. R., & Newsome, W. T. (1987). Visual processing in monkey extrastriate cortex. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 10, 363–401.
Mayer, R. E. (1977). Thinking and problem solving. Scott, Foresman & Company.
Mays, W. (1953). The first circuit for an electrical logic-machine. Science, 118(3062), 281–282.
McBeath, M. K., Shaffer, D. M., & Kaiser, M. K. (1995). How baseball outfielders determine where to run to catch fly balls. Science, 268(5210), 569–573. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.7725104
McBride, D. M., & Cutting, J. C. (2019). Cognitive psychology: Theory, process, and methodology (2nd ed.). SAGE Publications.
McClelland, J. L. (1979). Time relations of mental processes: Examination of systems of processes in cascade. Psychological Review, 86(4), 287–330. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295x.86.4.287
McClelland, J. L., & Rumelhart, D. E. (1986). Parallel distributed processing. Volume 2: Psychological and biological models. MIT Press.
McCloskey, M. (1991). Networks and theories: The place of connectionism in cognitive science. Psychological Science, 2, 387–395.
McCorduck, P. (1979). Machines who think: A personal inquiry into the history and prospects of artificial intelligence. W. H. Freeman.
McCulloch, W. S., & Pitts, W. (1943). A logical calculus of the ideas immanent in nervous activity. Bulletin of Mathematical Biophysics, 5, 115–133.
McGeoch, J. A. (1942). The psychology of human learning, an introduction. Longmans, Green and Company.
Mechanic, A. (1964). Responses involved in rote learning of verbal materials. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 3(1), 30–36. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0022-5371(64)80056-6
Medin, D. L., & Ross, B. H. (1992). Cognitive psychology. Harcourt Brace Jovanovich College Publishers.
Medler, D. A. (1998). A brief history of connectionism. Neural Computing Surveys, 1, 18–72.
Medler, D. A., Dawson, M. R. W., & Kingstone, A. (2005). Functional localization and double dissociations: The relationship between internal structure and behavior. Brain and Cognition, 57, 146–150.
Menary, R. (2008). Cognitive integration: Mind and cognition unbounded. Palgrave Macmillan.
Menary, R. (2010). The extended mind. MIT Press.
Menzel, R., & Fischer, J. (2012). Animal thinking: Contemporary issues in comparative cognition. MIT Press.
Meyer, D. E., Glass, J. M., Mueller, S. T., Seymour, T. L., & Kieras, D. E. (2001). Executive-process interactive control: A unified computational theory for answering 20 questions (and more) about cognitive ageing. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 13(1–2), 123–164.
Meyer, D. E., & Kieras, D. E. (1997a). A computational theory of executive cognitive processes and multiple-task performance. 1. Basic mechanisms. Psychological Review, 104(1), 3–65.
Meyer, D. E., & Kieras, D. E. (1997b). A computational theory of executive cognitive processes and multiple-task performance. 2. Accounts of psychological refractory-period phenomena. Psychological Review, 104(4), 749–791.
Miller, G. A. (1951). Language and communication. McGraw-Hill.
Miller, G. A. (1956). The magical number seven, plus or minus two: Some limits on our capacity for processing information. Psychological Review, 63(2), 81–97. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0043158
Miller, G. A. (2003). The cognitive revolution: A historical perspective. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(3), 141–144.
Miller, G. A., Galanter, E., & Pribram, K. H. (1960). Plans and the structure of behavior. Henry Holt & Company.
Milner, B., Squire, L. R., & Kandel, E. R. (1998). Cognitive neuroscience and the study of memory. Neuron, 20(3), 445–468. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0896-6273(00)80987-3
Milner, P. M. (1957). The cell assembly: Mark II. Psychological Review, 64(4), 242–252.
Minsky, M. L. (1985). The society of mind. Simon & Schuster.
Minsky, M. L. (2006). The emotion machine: Commensense thinking, artificial intelligence, and the future of the human mind. Simon & Schuster.
Minsky, M. L., & Papert, S. (1969). Perceptrons: An introduction to computational geometry. MIT Press.
Mohamed, A., Dahl, G. E., & Hinton, G. E. (2012). Acoustic modeling using deep belief networks. IEEE Transactions on Audio Speech and Language Processing, 20(1), 14–22. https://doi.org/10.1109/tasl.2011.2109382
Mollon, J. D. (1982). Colour vision and colour blindness. In H. B. Barlow & J. D. Mollon (Eds.), The senses (pp. 165–191). Cambridge University Press.
Moore, T. V. (1939). Cognitive psychology. Lippincott.
Moravec, H. (1999). Robot. Oxford University Press.
Morris, R. G. M., Anderson, E., Lynch, G. S., & Baudry, M. (1986). Selective impairment of learning and blockade of long-term potentiation by an N-methyl-D-aspartate receptor antagonist, AP5. Nature, 319(6056), 774–776. https://doi.org/10.1038/319774a0
Mozer, M. C., & Smolensky, P. (1989). Using relevance to reduce network size automatically. Connection Science, 1, 3–16.
Murdock, B. B. (1962). The serial position effect of free recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 64(5), 482–488. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0045106
Neisser, U. (1967). Cognitive psychology. Appleton-Century-Crofts.
Newell, A. (1973). Production systems: Models of control structures. In W. G. Chase (Ed.), Visual information processing (pp. 463–526). Academic Press.
Newell, A. (1980). Physical symbol systems. Cognitive Science, 4, 135–183.
Newell, A. (1990). Unified theories of cognition. Harvard University Press.
Newell, A., Shaw, J. C., & Simon, H. A. (1958). Elements of a theory of human problem solving. Psychological Review, 65, 151–166.
Newell, A., & Simon, H. A. (1956). The logic theory machine: A complex information-processing system. IRE Transactions on Information Theory, 2(3), 61–79. https://doi.org/10.1109/tit.1956.1056797
Newell, A., & Simon, H. A. (1961). Computer simulation of human thinking. Science, 134(349), 2011–2017.
Newell, A., & Simon, H. A. (1972). Human problem solving. Prentice-Hall.
Newell, A., & Simon, H. A. (1976). Computer science as empirical inquiry—Symbols and search. Communications of the ACM, 19(3), 113–126.
Nicholls, J. G., Martin, A. R., & Wallace, B. G. (1992). From neuron to brain (3rd ed.). Sinauer Associates.
Nilsson, N. J. (1984). Shakey the robot. Stanford Research Institute.
Nilsson, N. J. (2010). The quest for artificial intelligence: A history of ideas and achievements. Cambridge University Press.
Noble, C. E. (1952). An analysis of meaning. Psychological Review, 59(6), 421–430. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0054087
Noë, A. (2004). Action in perception. MIT Press.
Noë, A. (2009). Out of our heads. Hill and Wang.
Olmstead, M. C., & Kuhlmeier, V. A. (2015). Comparative cognition. Cambridge University Press.
Osbeck, L. M. (2019). Values in psychological science: Re-imagining epistemic priorities at a new frontier. Cambridge University Press.
Paivio, A. (1969). Mental imagery in associative learning and memory. Psychological Review, 76, 241–263.
Paivio, A. (1971). Imagery and verbal processes. Holt, Rinehart & Winston.
Paivio, A. (1986). Mental representations: A dual-coding approach. Oxford University Press.
Paivio, A., & Csapo, K. (1969). Concrete image and verbal memory codes. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 80(2P1), 279–285. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0027273
Paivio, A., Smythe, P. C., & Yuille, J. C. (1968). Imagery versus meaningfulness of nouns in paired-associate learning. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 22(6), 427–441. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0082782
Paivio, A., Yuille, J. C., & Madigan, S. A. (1968). Concreteness imagery and meaningfulness values for 925 nouns. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 76(1 Monograph Supplement), 1–25. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0025327
Papert, S. (1988). One AI or many? Daedalus, 117(1), 1–14.
Pedersen, D. B. (2011). Revisiting the neuro-turn in the humanities and natural sciences. Pensamiento, 67(254), 767–786.
Pennington, L. A., & Waters, R. H. (1938). The anticipation method in paired associate learning. Journal of Psychology, 6(2), 281–283. https://doi.org/10.1080/00223980.1938.9917605
Peterson, L. R., & Peterson, M. J. (1959). Short-term retention of individual verbal items. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58, 193–198.
Pinker, S. (1979). Formal models of language learning. Cognition, 7, 217–283.
Pinker, S. (1997). How the mind works. W. W. Norton.
Pinker, S., & Prince, A. (1988). On language and connectionism: Analysis of a parallel distributed processing model of language acquisition. Cognition, 28, 73–193.
Polger, T. W. (2012). Functionalism as a philosophical theory of the cognitive sciences. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 3(3), 337–348. https://doi.org/10.1002/wcs.1170
Polger, T. W., & Shapiro, L. A. (2016). The multiple realization book. Oxford University Press.
Popper, K. (1978). Natural selection and the emergence of mind. Dialectica, 32, 339–355.
Posner, M. I. (1978). Chronometric explorations of mind. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Posner, M. I., & Snyder, C. R. R. (1975). Attention and cognitive control. In R. L. Solso (Ed.), Information processing and cognition: The Loyola symposium (pp. 55–85). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Putnam, H. (1975a). The nature of mental states. In H. Putnam (Ed.), Mind, language and reality: Philosophical papers (Vol. 2, pp. 51–58). Cambridge University Press.
Putnam, H. (1975b). Philosophy and our mental life. In H. Putnam (Ed.), Mind, language and reality: Philosophical papers (Vol. 2, pp. 291–303). Cambridge University Press.
Pylyshyn, Z. W. (1973). What the mind’s eye tells the mind’s brain: A critique of mental imagery. Psychological Bulletin, 80, 1–24.
Pylyshyn, Z. W. (1980). Computation and cognition: Issues in the foundations of cognitive science. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 3(1), 111–132.
Pylyshyn, Z. W. (1981a). The imagery debate: Analogue media versus tacit knowledge. Psychological Review, 88(1), 16–45.
Pylyshyn, Z. W. (1981b). Psychological explanations and knowledge-dependent processes. Cognition, 10(1–3), 267–274.
Pylyshyn, Z. W. (1984). Computation and cognition. MIT Press.
Pylyshyn, Z. W. (1987). The robot’s dilemma: The frame problem in artificial intelligence. Ablex.
Pylyshyn, Z. W. (2001). Visual indexes, preconceptual objects, and situated vision. Cognition, 80(1–2), 127–158.
Pylyshyn, Z. W. (2003a). Seeing and visualizing: It’s not what you think. MIT Press.
Pylyshyn, Z. W. (2003b). Explaining mental imagery: Now you see it, now you don’t—Reply to Kosslyn et al. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(3), 111–112.
Pylyshyn, Z. W. (2003c). Return of the mental image: Are there really pictures in the brain? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(3), 113–118.
Pylyshyn, Z. W. (2007). Things and places: How the mind connects with the world. MIT Press.
Pylyshyn, Z. W., Haladjian, H. H., King, C. E., & Reilly, J. E. (2008). Selective nontarget inhibition in Multiple Object Tracking. Visual Cognition, 16(8), 1011–1021. https://doi.org/10.1080/13506280802247486
Quillian, M. R. (1967). Word concepts: A theory and simulation of some basic semantic capabilities. Behavioral Science, 12(5), 410–430. https://doi.org/10.1002/bs.3830120511
Quillian, M. R. (1969). Teachable language comprehender: A simulation program and theory of language. Communications of the ACM, 12(8), 459–476. https://doi.org/10.1145/363196.363214
Ratcliff, R. (1990). Connectionist models of recognition memory—Constraints imposed by learning and forgetting functions. Psychological Review, 97(2), 285–308.
Reed, S. K. (1982). Cognition: Theory and applications. Brooks/Cole.
Reed, S. K. (1988). Cognition: Theory and applications (2nd ed.). Brooks/Cole.
Reed, S. K. (1996). Cognition: Theory and applications. Wadsworth Publishing.
Reisberg, D. (2013). Cognition: Exploring the science of the mind (5th ed.). W. W. Norton & Company.
Reisberg, D. (2018). Cognition: Exploring the science of the mind (7th ed.). W. W. Norton & Company.
Reitman, W. R. (1965). Cognition and thought: An information processing approach. John Wiley & Sons.
Restle, F., & Greeno, J. G. (1970). Introduction to mathematical psychology. Addison-Wesley.
Reynolds, A. G., & Flagg, P. W. (1977). Cognitive psychology. Winthrop Publishers.
Rips, L., Shoben, E. J., & Smith, E. E. (1973). Semantic distance and verification of semantic relations. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 12, 1–20.
Rizzuto, D. S., & Kahana, M. J. (2001). An autoassociative neural network model of paired-associate learning. Neural Computation, 13(9), 2075–2092.
Robbins, P., & Aydede, M. (2009). The Cambridge handbook of situated cognition. Cambridge University Press.
Rochester, N., Holland, J. H., Haibt, L. H., & Duda, W. L. (1956). Tests on a cell assembly theory of the action of the brain, using a large digital computer. IRE Transactions on Information Theory, IT-2, 80–93.
Rock, I. (1983). The logic of perception. MIT Press.
Rosch, E. (1975). Cognitive reference points. Cognitive Psychology, 7(4), 532–547.
Rosch, E., & Mervis, C. B. (1975). Family resemblances: Studies in the internal structure of categories. Cognitive Psychology, 7, 573–605.
Rosenblatt, F. (1958). The perceptron: A probabilistic model for information storage and organization in the brain. Psychological Review, 65(6), 386–408.
Rosenblatt, F. (1962). Principles of neurodynamics. Spartan Books.
Rowlands, M. (2010). The new science of the mind: From extended mind to embodied phenomenology. MIT Press.
Rumelhart, D. E., Hinton, G. E., & Williams, R. J. (1986). Learning representations by back-propagating errors. Nature, 323(6088), 533–536.
Rumelhart, D. E., & McClelland, J. L. (1986a). On learning the past tenses of English verbs. In J. L. McClelland & D. E. Rumelhart (Eds.), Parallel distributed processing, V.2. (pp. 216–271). MIT Press.
Rumelhart, D. E., & McClelland, J. L. (1986b). Parallel distributed processing, V.1. MIT Press.
Rundus, D. (1971). Analysis of rehearsal processes in free recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 89(1), 63–77. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0031185
Rupert, R. D. (2009). Cognitive systems and the extended mind. Oxford University Press.
Ryle, G. (1949). The concept of mind. Hutchinson & Company.
Samuel, A. L. (1959). Some studies in machine learning using the game of checkers. IBM Journal of Research and Development, 3(3), 211–229. https://doi.org/10.1147/rd.33.0210
Sarikaya, R., Hinton, G. E., & Deoras, A. (2014). Application of deep belief networks for natural language understanding. IEEE-ACM Transactions on Audio Speech and Language Processing, 22(4), 778–784. https://doi.org/10.1109/taslp.2014.2303296
Saxberg, B. V. H. (1987a). Projected free fall trajectories. 1. Theory and simulation. Biological Cybernetics, 56(2–3), 159–175. https://doi.org/10.1007/bf00317991
Saxberg, B. V. H. (1987b). Projected free fall trajectories. 2. Human experiments. Biological Cybernetics, 56(2–3), 177–184. https://doi.org/10.1007/bf00317992
Schaeffer, J., Culberson, J., Treloar, N., Knight, B., Lu, P., & Szafron, D. (1992). A world championship caliber checkers program. Artificial Intelligence, 53(2–3), 273–289. https://doi.org/10.1016/0004-3702(92)90074-8
Schaeffer, J., Lake, R., Lu, P., & Bryant, M. (1995). Chinook: The world man-machine checkers champion. AI Magazine, 17(1), 21–29.
Schaeffer, J., Treloar, N., Lu, P., & Lake, R. (1993). Man versus machine . . . for the world checkers championship. AI Magazine, 14(2), 28–35.
Schlimmer, J. S. (1987). Concept acquisition through representational adjustment (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). University of California Irvine.
Schnapf, J. L., Nunn, B. J., Meister, M., & Baylor, D. A. (1990). Visual transductions in cones of the monkey Macaca fascicularis. Journal of Physiology, 427, 681–713.
Schneider, W. (1987). Connectionism: Is it a paradigm shift for psychology? Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 19, 73–83.
Schneider, W., & Shiffrin, R. M. (1977). Controlled and automatic human information-processing. 1. Detection, search, and attention. Psychological Review, 84(1), 1–66. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295x.84.1.1
Schriber, T. J. (1969). Fundamentals of flowcharting. Wiley.
Scoville, W. B., & Milner, B. (1957). Loss of recent memory after bilateral hippocampal lesions. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 20, 11–21.
Scribner, S., & Tobach, E. (1997). Mind and social practice: Selected writings of Sylvia Scribner. Cambridge University Press.
Searle, J. R. (1980). Minds, brains, and programs. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 3, 417–424.
Searle, J. R. (1984). Minds, brains and science. Harvard University Press.
Searle, J. R. (1990). Is the brain’s mind a computer program? Scientific American, 262, 26–31.
Searle, J. R. (2007). Putting consciousness back in the brain: Reply to Bennett and Hacker. In M. R. Bennett, D. C. Dennett, P. M. S. Hacker, & J. R. Searle (Eds.), Neuroscience and philosophy: Brain, mind, and language (pp. 97–126). Columbia University Press.
Seidenberg, M. (1993). Connectionist models and cognitive theory. Psychological Science, 4, 228–235.
Shannon, C. E. (1938). A symbolic analysis of relay and switching circuits. Transactions of the American Institute of Electrical Engineers, 57, 713–723.
Shapiro, L. A. (2011). Embodied cognition. Routledge.
Shapiro, L. A. (2014). The Routledge handbook of embodied cognition. Routledge.
Shapiro, L. A. (2019). Embodied cognition (2nd ed.). Routledge.
Shen, D. G., Wu, G. R., & Suk, H. I. (2017). Deep learning in medical image analysis. Annual Review of Biomedical Engineering, 19(1), 221–248.
Shepard, R. N., & Cooper, L. A. (1982). Mental images and their transformations. MIT Press.
Shepard, R. N., & Metzler, J. (1971). Mental rotation of three-dimensional objects. Science, 171(3972), 701–703.
Shettleworth, S. J. (2013). Fundamentals of comparative cognition. Oxford University Press.
Shiffrin, R. M., & Atkinson, R. C. (1969). Storage and retrieval processes in long-term memory. Psychological Review, 76(2), 179–193.
Shiffrin, R. M., & Schneider, W. (1977). Controlled and automatic human information-processing. 2. Perceptual learning, automatic attending, and a general theory. Psychological Review, 84(2), 127–190. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295x.84.2.127
Simon, H. A. (1969). The sciences of the artificial. MIT Press.
Simon, H. A. (1979). Models of thought. Yale University Press.
Simon, H. A. (2007). Karl Duncker and cognitive science. In J. Valsiner (Ed.), Thinking in psychological science: Ideas and their makers (pp. 3–16). Transaction Publishers.
Simon, H. A., & Newell, A. (1958). Heuristic problem solving: The next advance in operations research. Operations Research, 6, 1–10.
Simpson, D. (2005). Phrenology and the neurosciences: Contributions of F. J. Gall and J. G. Spurzheim. ANZ Journal of Surgery, 75(6), 475–482. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1445-2197.2005.03426.x
Sinnett, S., Smilek, D., & Kingstone, A. (2016). Cognition (6th ed.). Oxford University Press.
Skinner, B. F. (1950). Are theories of learning necessary? Psychological Review, 57(4), 193–216. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0054367
Skinner, B. F. (1957). Verbal behavior. Appleton-Century-Crofts.
Skinner, B. F. (1977). Why I am not a cognitive psychologist. Behaviorism, 5(2), 1–10.
Skinner, B. F. (1990). Can psychology be a science of mind? American Psychologist, 45(11), 1206–1210. https://doi.org/10.1037//0003-066x.45.11.1206
Smolensky, P. (1988). On the proper treatment of connectionism. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 11, 1-74.
Solso, R. L. (1979). Cognitive psychology. Harcourt, Brace, Jovanovich.
Solso, R. L. (1988). Cognitive psychology (2nd ed.). Allyn & Bacon.
Solso, R. L. (1995). Cognitive psychology (4th ed.). Allyn & Bacon.
Sorabji, R. (2006). Aristotle on memory (2nd ed.). University of Chicago Press.
Sperling, G. (1960). The information available in brief visual presentations. Psychological Monographs, 74(11), 1–29. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0093759
Sperry, R. W. (1993). The impact and promise of the cognitive revolution. American Psychologist, 48(8), 878–885.
Squire, L. R. (1987). Memory and brain. Oxford University Press.
Squire, L. R. (1992). Declarative and nondeclarative memory: Multiple brain systems supporting learning and memory. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 4, 232–243.
Squire, L. R. (2009). The legacy of patient H. M. for neuroscience. Neuron, 61(1), 6–9. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2008.12.023
Squire, L. R., & Wixted, J. T. (2011). The cognitive neuroscience of human memory since HM., Annual Review of Neuroscience, 34(1) 259–288.
Sternberg, S. (1969a). Discovery of processing stages: Extensions of Donders’ method. Acta Psychologica, 30, 276–315. https://doi.org/10.1016/0001-6918(69)90055-9
Sternberg, S. (1969b). Memory-scanning: Mental processes revealed by reaction-time experiments. American Scientist, 4, 421–457.
Stewart, I. (1994). A subway named Turing. Scientific American, 271, 104–107.
Stone, J. L., & Hughes, J. R. (2013). Early history of electroencephalography and establishment of the American Clinical Neurophysiology Society. Journal of Clinical Neurophysiology, 30(1), 28–44. https://doi.org/10.1097/WNP.0b013e31827edb2d
Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18, 643–662. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.121.1.15
Surprenant, A. M., & Neath, I. (1997). T. V. Moore’s (1939) Cognitive psychology. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 4(3), 342–349. https://doi.org/10.3758/bf03210791
Swade, D. D. (1993). Redeeming Charles Babbage’s mechanical computer. Scientific American, 268, 86–91.
Theraulaz, G., & Bonabeau, E. (1999). A brief history of stigmergy. Artificial Life, 5, 97–116.
Thorndike, E. L. (1932). The fundamentals of learning. Bureau of Publications, Teachers College, Columbia University.
Todd, P. M., & Loy, D. G. (1991). Music and connectionism. MIT Press.
Townsend, J. T. (1971). Note on identifiability of parallel and serial processes. Perception & Psychophysics, 10(3), 161–163.
Townsend, J. T. (1990). Serial vs parallel processing: Sometimes they look like Tweedledum and Tweedledee but they can (and should) be distinguished. Psychological Science, 1(1), 46–54.
Treisman, A. M. (1985). Preattentive processing in vision. Computer Vision, Graphics, and Image Processing, 31, 156–177.
Treisman, A. M. (1986). Features and objects in visual processing. Scientific American, 254, 114–124.
Treisman, A. M. (1988). Features and objects: The fourteenth Bartlett memorial lecture. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 40A, 201–237.
Treisman, A. M., & Gelade, G. (1980). A feature integration theory of attention. Cognitive Psychology, 12(1), 97–136.
Treisman, A. M., & Gormican, S. (1988). Feature analysis in early vision: Evidence from search asymmetries. Psychological Review, 95, 14–48.
Treisman, A. M., Sykes, M., & Gelade, G. (1977). Selective attention and stimulus integration. In S. Dornic (Ed.), Attention and performance VI. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Tschentscher, N., Hauk, O., Fischer, M. H., & Pulvermuller, F. (2012). You can count on the motor cortex: Finger counting habits modulate motor cortex activation evoked by numbers. Neuroimage, 59(4), 3139–3148. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.11.037
Tulving, E. (1983). Elements of episodic memory. Oxford University Press.
Tulving, E., Donaldson, W., & Bower, G. H. (1972). Organization of memory. Academic Press.
Turing, A. M. (1936). On computable numbers, with an application to the Entscheidungsproblem. Proceedings of the London Mathematical Society, Series 2h, 42, 230–265.
Turing, A. M. (1950). Computing machinery and intelligence. Mind, 59, 433–460.
Turner, B. M., Miletic, S., & Forstmann, B. U. (2018). Outlook on deep neural networks in computational cognitive neuroscience. Neuroimage, 180, 117–118. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.12.078
Tversky, A. (1977). Features of similarity. Psychological Review, 84, 327–352.
Tye, M. (1991). The imagery debate. MIT Press.
Uexküll, J. V. (1957). A stroll through the worlds of animals and men: A picture book of invisible worlds. In C. H. Schiller (Ed.), Instinctive behavior: The development of a modern concept (Vol. 134, pp. 5–80). International Universities Press.
Uexküll, J. V. (2001). An introduction to umwelt. Semiotica, 134(1–4), 107–110.
Underwood, B. J., & Schulz, R. W. (1960). Meaningfulness and verbal learning. Lippincott.
Ungerleider, L. G., & Mishkin, M. (1982). Two cortical visual systems. In D. Ingle, M. A. Goodale, & R. J. W. Mansfield (Eds.), Analysis of visual behavior (pp. 549–586). MIT Press.
Uttal, W. R. (2011). Mind and brain: A critical appraisal of cognitive neuroscience. MIT Press.
van Essen, D. C., Anderson, C. H., & Felleman, D. J. (1992). Information processing in the primate visual system: An integrated systems perspective. Science, 255(5043), 419–423.
van Hemmen, J. L., & Senn, W. (2002). Hebb in perspective. Biological Cybernetics, 87, 317–318.
VanLehn, K. (1991). Architectures for intelligence. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Varela, F. J., Thompson, E., & Rosch, E. (1991). The embodied mind: Cognitive science and human experience. MIT Press.
Vico, G. (1710/1988). On the most ancient wisdom of the Italians. Ithaca: Cornell University Press.
Vidal, F., & Ortega, F. (2017). Being brains: Making the cerebral subject. Fordham University Press.
Virues-Ortega, J. (2006). The case against B. F. Skinner 45 years later: An encounter with N. Chomsky. Behavior Analyst, 29(2), 243–251. https://doi.org/10.1007/bf03392133
von Neumann, J. (1958). The computer and the brain. Yale University Press.
Vygotsky, L. S. (1986). Thought and language (Translation newly rev. and ed.). MIT Press.
Warren, H. C. (1921). A history of the association psychology. Charles Scribner’s Sons.
Wasner, M., Moeller, K., Fischer, M. H., & Nuerk, H. C. (2014). Aspects of situated cognition in embodied numerosity: The case of finger counting. Cognitive Processing, 15(3), 317–328. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10339-014-0599-z
Wason, P. C. (1966). Reasoning. Penguin.
Wason, P. C., & Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1972). Psychology of reasoning: Structure and content. Batsford.
Wasserman, G. S. (1978). Color vision: An historical introduction. John Wiley & Sons.
Watson, J. B. (1913). Psychology as the behaviorist views it. Psychological Review, 20, 158–177.
Waugh, N. C., & Norman, D. A. (1965). Primary memory. Psychological Review, 72, 89–104.
Weizenbaum, J. (1966). Eliza—A computer program for the study of natural language communication between man and machine. Communications of the ACM, 9(1), 36–45.
Weizenbaum, J. (1976). Computer power and human reason. W. H. Freeman.
Wertheimer, M., & Asch, S. E. (1945). Productive thinking. Harper & Brothers.
Wheeler, W. M. (1911). The ant colony as an organism. Journal of Morphology, 22(2), 307–325.
Widrow, B., & Hoff, M. E. (1960). Adaptive switching circuits. Institute of Radio Engineers, Wester Electronic Show and Convention, Convention Record, Part 4, 96–104.
Wiener, N. (1948). Cybernetics: Or control and communication in the animal and the machine. MIT Press.
Wiener, N. (1950). The human use of human beings: Cybernetics and society. Houghton Mifflin.
Wilkins, A. J. (1971). Conjoint frequency, category size, and categorization time. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 10(4), 382–385. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0022-5371(71)80036-1
Wilson, M. (2002). Six views of embodied cognition. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9(4), 625–636. https://doi.org/10.3758/bf03196322
Wilson, R. A. (2004). Boundaries of the mind: The individual in the fragile sciences—Cognition. Cambridge University Press.
Wilson, R. A. (2005). Genes and the agents of life: The individual in the fragile sciences—Biology. Cambridge University Press.
Winograd, T. (1972). Understanding natural language. Academic Press.
Winograd, T. (1983). Language as a cognitive process. Volume 1: Syntax. Addison-Wesley.
Winograd, T., & Flores, F. (1987). Understanding computers and cognition. Addison-Wesley.
Wittgenstein, L. (1953). Philosophical investigations (G. E. M. Anscombe, Trans.). Blackwell.
Wolfe, J. M. (1994). Guided search 2.0: A revised model of visual search. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 1(2), 202–238.
Wolfe, J. M., Cave, K. R., & Franzel, S. L. (1989). Guided search: An alternative to the feature integration model for visual search. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 15(3), 419–433.
Wolfe, J. M., Franzel, S. L., & Cave, K. R. (1988). Parallel visual search for conjunctions of color and form. Journal of the Optical Society of America, 4, 95.
Wood, G. (2002). Living dolls: A magical history of the quest for artificial life. Faber & Faber.
Woodman, G. F. (2010). A brief introduction to the use of event-related potentials in studies of perception and attention. Attention Perception & Psychophysics, 72(8), 2031–2046. https://doi.org/10.3758/app.72.8.2031
Woodworth, R. S. (1938). Experimental psychology. H. Holt & Company.
Yates, F. A. (1966). The art of memory. University of Chicago Press.
We use cookies to analyze our traffic. Please decide if you are willing to accept cookies from our website. You can change this setting anytime in Privacy Settings.